Development of vocal folds from infancy to Puberty




The larynx and all parts of the vocal tract are changing throughout life and the voice reflects these changes. Some vocal features are the result of gross alterations such as the size of the vocal folds and the dimensions of the vocal tract structure, but there are the result of more subtle changes, such as the changing histology of the vocal folds and the timing of the neuronal impulses that initiate and regulate phonation.
The structure and the controlling mechanism of the phonation are in a process of maturation for the first 20 years of life. From embryological stage to old age, maturation and subsequent decline of the anatomy, physiology and histology of the vocal tract and its related systems results in acoustic changes in the voice. Throughout the lifespan the voice is affected by a number of internal and external factors which are tabulated as follows.
Factors affecting the voice from infancy to senescence
·         Growth, eg: bones, muscles, cartilages.
·         Hormones, especially androgens and oestrogens
·         Health: good, poor
·         Use: occupation and leisure activities
·         Lifestyle, eg: busy, relaxed, smoking, excessive alcohol, gastric reflux
·         Environment, eg: quite, pleasant, noisy, dusty, excessively dry atmosphere
·         Psychological status: eg: happiness, serenity, depression, anxiety, stress
·         Cultural factors: eg: vocally loud social/ family group
·         Degenerative changes, eg: bones, muscles, cartilages, nervous system, and respiratory system.
PRENATAL DEVELOPMENT
·         The respiratory primordium begins at about the fourth week of gestation with the formation of the laryngotracheal groove, which extends lengthwise in the floor of the gut just caudal to the pharyngeal pouches.
·         This groove deepens into the laryngotracheal diverticulum, whose ventral endoderm will become the larynx and trachea, while the more caudal endoderm will give rise to the bronchi and lungs. Lateral furrows develop on either side of the laryngotracheal groove or diverticulum, at the level of the junction between the groove and the future esophagus.
·         These furrows gradually deepen and extend cephalad, elongating this structure to form the primitive laryngotracheal tube. The lateral furrows join, splitting off first the lung bud and then the trachea.
·         The upper end of this tube advances slightly cephalad until it lies between the fourth branchial arches, and this region will form the primitive laryngeal aditus.
·         A tracheoesophageal septum develops caudally too cranially and separates the respiratory system from the esophagus.
·         It is at the end of the fourth week of gestation that the single lung bud appears at the caudal end of the laryngotracheal tube. This bud soon divides into right and left bronchial buds, and these grow caudally and laterally into the pericardioperitoneal cavities. The primitive lung buds then subdivide into secondary and tertiary bronchi. Throughout fetal development, the surrounding pulmogenic mesoderm continues to develop into the lung parenchyma.
·         The larynx develops on the proximal end of the laryngotracheal groove between the fifth and sixth gestational weeks, as three swellings appear at the laryngeal aditus. The anterior swelling, which is the future epiglottis, may be a derivative of the hypobranchial eminence from the fourth arch, although its actual origin and relationships to the branchial arches remain unclear. It should be noted that in contrast to the first, second, and third arches, little is clearly known about the precise development of the caudal arches, the fourth through sixth arches. The two lateral masses give rise to the future arytenoids, from the fourth or fifth-sixth arches. These two lateral swellings migrate cranially and medially to oppose each other and, together with the epiglottic swelling, surround a T-shaped laryngeal aditus.
·         The laryngeal lumen becomes occluded at eight weeks of gestation due to epithelial proliferation. If normal recanalization does not occur during the tenth gestational week, a laryngeal web results. The formation of the vocal (true) and vestibular (false) folds (cords) is related to the condensation of mesenchyme and the outpouching of the laryngeal sinus or ventricle.
·         The two vocal folds separate during the third gestational month and failure of this recanalization process results in congenital atresia of the larynx.

·         The laryngeal cartilages develop from the branchial arches, with the more cranial cartilages possibly arising from the fourth arch and the more caudal ones from the sixth (with the caveat that the epiglottis may not be arch derived, as mentioned above) . The thyroid cartilage develops from the fourth arch as two lateral plates that fuse in the midline. This process is almost completed by the ninth gestational week. The cricoid cartilage appears to begin as two cartilaginous centers of the sixth arch. First, the centers grow and unite in the ventral midline; then, by the seventh gestational week, they fuse dorsally. The rostral advancement of the tracheoesophageal septum results in the fusion of the dorsal cricoid lamina. Failure of advancement of this septum results in a fistula. At first, the cricoid lumen is slit-like in shape, but eventually the ventral and lateral walls of the cricoid cartilage condense and there is progressive enlargement of the lumen. Failure of this condensation process results in congenital subglottic stenosis.

·         The arytenoid cartilages develop from the arytenoids swellings, most likely derivatives of the sixth arch but possibly arising from the fourth arch. They are initially fused to the cricoid cartilage, but they eventually separate from it and form the cricoarytenoid joints. The origins of the corniculate and cuneiform cartilages remain unclear. The intrinsic laryngeal muscles develop from the mesoderm of the fourth through sixth arches. The tracheal tube elongates, and the future carina or point of bifurcation descends caudally eight somite segments. The right bronchus descends more directly than the left bronchus, a relationship that is maintained in the adult. The smooth muscle fibers and the cartilaginous tracheal rings differentiate from the surrounding mesenchyme at the end of the seventh week. The minor salivary glands develop as ingrowths from the epithelium after four months of gestation.

                  

1.    
           
                                                           
INFANCY
Ø  Upper respiratory tract
 Laryngeal position
The configuration of the infant aerodigestive region is radically different from adult aerodigestive region. At birth, the larynx is high in the vocal tract with the lower border of the cricoids cartilage at the level of cervical vertebrae 3 and 4 (C3-C4) (Maddern, Campbell and Stool, 1991). The tip of the epiglottis is parallel with the upper portion of the body of C1 and in some cases it is in contact with the soft palate. This arrangement allows the infant to breathe and swallow almost simultaneously, as in some other mammals. The root of the tongue is in the oral cavity but during the first 4 years of life, the larynx and the root of the tongue descend into pharynx (most of the descent occurs in infancy). The vocal tract above larynx is also primitive in development and restricted in resonance variability. The narrowest point of the airway in the neonate is the subglottis within the cricoids ring, whereas in the adult the narrowest point is the glottis itself (Tucker, 1987). It is estimated that, in about 50% of infants, the epiglottis is omega or U-shaped. At this early stage of life, the hyoid bone and the thyroid cartilage are closely approximated, but they gradually separate as the larynx descends in the vocal tract throughout childhood.

Vocal fold length
There is some difference of opinion concerning the exact length of the vocal folds at birth, but the most notable feature is the minute size of the laryngeal sphincter. Negus (1949) reported that the folds are 3 mm long at 14 days, 5.5 mm at 1 year; 7.5 mm at 5 years, 8.0 mm at 6 years 6 months, and 9.5 mm at 15 years .Hirano, Kurita and Nakashima (1983), in the examination of 88 Japanese infants, calculated that the length of vocal folds i the newborn varied from 2.5 mm to 3.0 mm. Von Leden (1961) and Hollien (1980) reported that the length of the vocal folds increases about 80% from birth to 12 months of age. Variance in size and eight of infants determines laryngeal size. More than 50% of the glottis opening in the infant is cartilaginous, in contrast to two-thirds of the glottis being bordered by soft tissue in the adult (Tucker, 1987)
Age
Vocal fold length
authors
Neonate             

2.5-3.0

Hirano, Kurita and Nakashima (1983)
14 days

3

Negus(1949)
1year

5.5

Negus(1949)
5 years

7.5

Negus(1949)
6 years 6 months

8

Negus (1949)
9 years

9

Mueller (1997)
Puberty

12-15

Mueller (1997)
Women

12-17

Mueller (1997)


17-23

Mueller (1997)

Vocal fold histology
The fibres of the vocalis muscle are incomplete at birth and develop along­side the thyroarytenoid muscle, which increases considerably in size from the ninth postnatal month. The mucosal cover of the vocal folds is very thick in relation to its length and there is no vocal ligament observable in early infancy. This develops between 1 and 4 years. The intermediate and deep layers of the lamina propria are not differentiated into collagenous and elastic fibres. The thick loose layer of the lamina propria is prone to develop acute oedema which is the cause of croup. Although the airway becomes constricted, total obstruction does not occur because the mem­branous length is almost the equivalent of the cartilaginous length of the fold (Hirano, 1981; Hirano, Kurita and Nakashima, 1983; Kahane, 1986). Hirano, Kurita and Nakashima (1983) provide an excellent set of histolog­ical pictures illustrating the differences in structure of the vocal folds at birth, in the child and in the adult.
Ø  Lower respiratory tract
Structure
Before birth, the lungs are yellowish and solid, tucked away in the back c e chest. Immediately the child is delivered, 'the tissues of the lung expand like the petals of a flower and the color changes to rose red (Thomson, 1976). This is because of the inrush of blood and air into the expanding lung tissues. From the dramatic moment of the birth cry, the infant is launched into automatic life-supporting respiration. The glottis is sphincteric and widens and narrows reflexly in concert with inspiration and expiration (Terracol, Guerrier and Camps, 1956). The pharynx is hypersensitive which ensures instant spasmodic closure of the glottis at the slightest excitation from saliva or milk. This is followed by immediate, expulsion by coughing and spluttering accompanied by poorly coordinate inspiratory and expiratory action which, however, proves effective. The diaphragm is the chief muscle involved in respiration in infancy. The ribs are relatively perpendicular to the spine and do not contribute to thoracic movement until the child is able to sit and assume upright posture. The act of crying necessitates changes in respiratory patterns and provides essential preliminary exercise for phonic respiration.
Function
During the first year, control over vegetative respiration gradually devel­ops. The infant acquires the ability to change from quiet breathing to the changed rhythm and volume necessary in vocalization, in babbling and eventually in speech. Study of respiratory movements can be registered by magnetometry, which tracks the anteroposterior diameter of the rib cage and abdominal wall. Impedance pneumography measures the circumfer­ence of the same structures. Such measurements are non-intrusive but accurate, and have revealed that breathing in infants is extremely variable. At 1 month, breaths may be taken at a rate of 87/min and irregularity is not uncommon (Perkins and Kent, 1986). The rate decreases gradually to 61 breaths at 6 months and 42 at 12 months (Langlois, Wilder and Baken-1975; Langlois, Baken and Wilder, 1980). As the infant grows and the laryngeal airway increases in size, the airflow increases and airway resistance decreases (Netsell et al., 1994).
Phonation
Infant cries                                                                                     
The voice is used to signal distress and discomfort and to emit cries for help. The first cry at birth is probably the most dramatic use of voice that an individual will ever make. It signals that the infant is alive and respiration has commenced. The primitive nature of the baby's vocal apparatus means that comparison with a musical instrument at this stage is invalid. The sounds emitted encompass a considerable range of frequencies and an amazing volume of sound considering the tiny size of the instrument (Murry, 1980; Raes and Dehaen, 1998). The vocal tract of the newborn is incapable of producing the full range of speech sounds, although the formants of vowels /a/ and /a/ are apparent in sound spectrographic analysis (Ringel and Kluppel, 1964; Stark, 1978) The formants produced inevitably reflect the characteristics of a vocal tract that, at birth, resembles that non-human primate more than that of human adult (Lieberman, 1967). The larynx is elevated in the vocal tract during crying.

An early study was carried out by Fairbanks (1942) on his son from the age of 1 to 9 months. He recorded the fundamental frequency of hunger wails. At 1 month, the mean fundamental frequency was 373 Hz and subsequently increased to a mean of 814 Hz at 5 months, and then stabilized, to a decreased mean of 640 Hz at 9 months. He attributed the reg­ular and rapid rise in frequency up to 5 months to increased neuromuscular development and not to increasing length of the vocal folds.

A study, by Sheppard and Lane (1968), of a male and female for the first 141 days of life showed that the fundamental frequency for the male baby's cry was 443 Hz with a range of 404-481 Hz. The mean for the female baby was 414 Hz with a range of 384-481 Hz. Ostwald (1963) emphasized that the fundamental frequency of a newborn infant's cry may fluctuate from 400 Hz to 600 Hz. Other studies have shown that a pitch range from 300 Hz to 800 Hz is possible. The cries of pain and hunger of the average neonate are produced within a frequency range rising to
500 Hz (Maddern, Campbell and Stool,1991). This accounts for the heart­rending and ear-splitting potential of the infant cry. The amplitude of these wails appears not to have engaged the interest of most researchers in the field. Langlois, Baken and Wilder (1980) comment on the fact that scant attention has been paid to the infant's development of respiratory control despite its relevance to the understanding of speech development.

It is generally believed that a mother is able to identify the cause of her baby's crying, whether hunger or pain, by its acoustic quality. However, it has been found that mothers could not identify cry samples correctly according to the cause of the cry. In a review of studies of perceptual iden­tification of cry types, Hollien (1980) concluded that cries actually contain insufficient perceptual information to identify the reason for crying. The cry attracts the mother's attention and is subsequently categorized by environ­mental clues. Increases in amplitude and duration of wails provide informa­tion regarding the degree of distress. However, a mother can identify the voice of her own infant crying. Valanne et al. (1967) examined the ability of mothers to identify the hunger cry of their own newborn infants during the lying-in period. They found that mothers were successful in identifying their own babies from an audiotape recording that included various other infants. Formby (1967) and Murry (1980) reported similar findings. The baby's cry has individual and personal characteristics, as one would expect from the fact that they have different physiognomies.
Vegetative sounds
Infants produce a range of non-crying sounds described as 'vegetative'. These include coughs, burps, hiccups, lip smacking and sucking, spitting, etc., accompanied by ingressive as well as ingressive breathing. The study of vegetative sounds is naturally less interesting than that of infant cries. The gradual emergence of 'comfort sounds', as Lewis (1936) described them, as distinct from discomfort cries, is the first step in the acquisition of speech. Vocal play and babbling are an important transitional stage of development leading into the prosodic features of speech.

Prelinguistic tonal development
Lewis (1936), in his classic study of infant speech, distinguished both dis­comfort and comfort sounds. He stressed the pleasure evinced by the baby in making and experimenting with musical vowel-like sounds, and also the pleasure shared by the mother and her response in encouraging these first elements of vocal communication. At 6 weeks an advanced baby's response to a strange face and voice is negative, but mother's face and voice evoke when she is out of sight. The first smile appears a couple of weeks later and coos, gurgles and little shrieks are produced, especially when the baby is spoken to and caressed (Weisberg, 1963; Stark, 1979; Illingworth, 1980). Mother and responsive childminders reinforce social reactions, especially by talking to the baby, all of which is crucial for normal emotional and speech development. The child therefore needs to be caressed and talked to when handled.

The comfort sounds increase steadily. A healthy sign is the variabil­ity and pitch range of the musical glides that are emitted. This indicates the maturation of the vocal folds and improved muscular coordination. Expiration is matched to phonation and control of breath groups subserves vocal expression (Lieberman, 1967). Throughout the developmental sequence cortical and neuromuscular maturation keep pace.
Upward glides appeared in these babies first, then rising and falling glides which increased in quantity and range progressively, covering approximately an octave at 6-7 months. The influence of heard speech, described by Piaget (1952) as 'contagion', is obviously strong in this devel­opment and is present as early as 1 month of age. The great versatility in vocal behavior is confirmed by Murry, Hoit Dalgaad and Gracco (1983) in their study of one child's hunger, discomfort and non-distress cries from 2 to 12 weeks of age. They clearly distinguished seven melody types in each of the three categories of vocalization. The rapid shifts and wide frequency range reflected increasing respiratory and phonatory control, exhibiting early communication behavior.

CHILDHOOD
Ø  Upper respiratory tract
From 4 years until puberty, the dimensions of the entire vocal tract are increasing in conjunction with improving neuromuscular coordination. The larynx of the 2-year-old child is at the level of the mid-portion of C5, and it continues to descend relatively rapidly until it is at the level of C 6 at the age of 5 years. It does not reach its adult position with the lower border of the cricoid cartilage at the level of the C6-C7 vertebral disc until the individual is aged 15 years (Maddern, Campbell and Stool, 1991).
Ø  Lower respiratory tract
Throughout childhood, there are developmental changes in the respiratory system and breathing for speech differs significantly from adults' speech breathing patterns. Four-year-old children, for example, exert far more expiratory effort in speech breathing than adults, and 7 year olds use relatively higher lung volumes to initiate vocal fold vibration than older chil­dren and adults (Netsell et al., 1994). Before puberty, lung function is almost identical in boys and girls of equal size. Boys' chests, however, grow in lateral and longitudinal dimensions more than those of girls. It is inter­esting to note that a low level of activity in childhood affects the size of the lungs. Cotes (1979) found that children living in high blocks of flats, where the opportunity for exercise was limited, had 7% less vital capacity than physically active children. This is a factor that must not be overlooked in measurements of airflow and phonation time in children.
Phonation
The fundamental frequency continues to decrease with age as the larynx enlarges and the vocal folds increase in mass (Robb and Saxman, 1985). By 5 years, the child's speaking voice settles at a median pitch in the region of middle C, or maybe two or three semitones higher. The child's singing range varies very little between "boys and girls at the age of 7 years (Tarneaud, 1961). At 8 years, the lower range is only slightly extended and by 9 years the range extends a little further in both directions from B2 to D4.

ADOLESCENCE
Ø  Upper respiratory tract
Laryngeal skeleton changes
At the onset of puberty and during the period from 10 to 14 years, there is a dramatic period of general growth associated with increased secretion of androgens in the male and oestrogens in female. As the hormonal changes take place, male and female secondary sexual characteristics emerge. The mutational period may be complete at 14 years in boys, but in girls it continues on average until 15 years. The dimensions of the vocal tract reflect this period of growth and differences between males and females. Laryngeal dimensions in the male are generally larger and the thy­roid cartilage changes its configuration. Until puberty, the angle of the thyroid cartilage is 120° in both males and females. During pubertal change in the male, the thyroid cartilage enlarges significantly and the angle, decreases to 90°, giving rise to the marked thyroid prominence known as the Adam's apple.

Vocal fold length
The increased size of the laryngeal skeleton is reflected in the length of the vocal folds. In girls, the mean length of the vocal folds is 15mm before puberty and this may increase to 17 mm in a contralto. During the mutational period, a boy’s vocal folds double in length and may increase to a maximum of 23 mm in the bass voice. The normal minimum vocal fold length for the male is 17 mm, so it can be seen that a tenor and a contralto may have much the same pitch range, but it is the larger resonators of the larynx, pharynx and the chest that distinguish the male from the female voice.

Vocal fold histology
The layer structure of the lamina propria of the vocal fold continues mature in adolescence and it is not until 16 years that it resembles the struc­ture of the adult vocal fold. Before this, the layers are less well defined. The change in the inner structure of the vocal fold mucosa is a significant factor in voice mutation, besides the increase in length of the vocal folds.

Ø  Lower respiratory tract
The young adult has approximately four times the lung volume of the 5 year old. Vital capacity is at its peak during the late teens and early 20s, after which it gradually deteriorates with reduced diaphragmatic action. Breathing rate rest is between 10 and 22 breaths/min (Perkins and Kent, 1986).
                                     
                
                                            
                             



Laryngeal cartilage enlarges,
laryngeal muscles enlarge, V.F lengthen, epiglottis enlarges,
flattens, elevates, neck elongates, larynx descends,
thorax enlarges, resonators enlarge.
 
   Post-puberty                                       
                                                                 18 years        
                                                                     SFo
                                                    137 Hz                        210 Hz


Phonation
Speaking fundamental frequency
As the vocal folds double in length in the male, the voice drops an octave whereas girl’s voices mature gradually as a result of the enlargement of the larynx, consistent with general body growth. Voice mutation and voice pitch are associated with the growth of the larynx and lengthening of the vocal folds. It is possible, however that the dramatically lowered speaking fundamental frequency in the male does not result entirely from the lengthening of the vocal folds, but also reflect changes in their mass and the developing differentiation of the histological layers (Harries et al., 1998). McGlone and Hollien (1963) found that a girl's vocal pitch is at its highest at 7-8 years, drops 2.4 semitones between 11 and 15 years, and remains at much the same level throughout life. Michel, Hollien and Moore (1966) recorded the speaking fundamental pitch of 15-, 16- and 17-year-old girls and found that this was 207 Hz. This indicates that fundamental frequency is established at 15 years in girls when pubertal mutation is over, although body growth continues up to 20 years of age and beyond.
Vocal shifts and breaks
Weiss (1950) defined 'break of voice' as a sudden and involuntary change in the pitch and quality. 'Voice break' should therefore be properly confined to the characteristic fluctuations in pitch and quality in adolescence during the period of voice mutation. The voice may rise or fall an octave, rising to falsetto or falling to the bass notes. The voice 'breaks' analyzed in the work of American workers described below refer either to the mutational period of voice break in adolescence or to 'shifts' in pitch during childhood. These shifts consist of abrupt and uncontrolled rises and falls in vocal pitch result­ing from poor coordination of the laryngeal musculature, associated with general bodily growth. In pre pubertal boys, these shifts do not have the masculine quality that is so conspicuous and bizarre a feature of the real break of voice in adolescence. The young boy's resonator system naturally cannot produce the necessary resonance characteristics of the adult male voice.
Vocal shifts appear to be a normal physiological feature of juvenile laryngeal function. These shifts may also be aggravated by vocal strain imposed by vocal abuse in children who shout and scream at football matches and in the playground. Vocal shifts and subharmonic breaks were recorded in infants by Wasz-Hockert et al. (1968). Fairbanks, Wiley and Lassman (1949) studied the voice breaks in voices of 7- and 8-year-old boys and girls. They concluded that the pitch changes recorded occurred as frequently in girls as in boys and were not sex linked or confined to ado­lescence.
Luchsinger (1962) stated that the real voice 'break' or 'stormy' mutation occurring in male adolescence is not the general rule and is encountered in only a minority of boys as a result of vocal or psychogenic strain. Weiss (1950) suggested that the sudden drop or rise in the voice, changing momentarily from the childish treble to the adult male voice or vice versa, is so conspicuous that it has accordingly been considered the main characteristic of the pubertal voice change, whereas it is actually uncharacteristic.
Singing in adolescence
In his book The Voice of the Boy (1919), Dawson attributed pitch breaks to collapse of the voice caused by misuse and vocal strain. None of his pupils suffered from 'breaking' of the speaking or singing voice. The boys' voices just slid down the scale. He evaluated the pitch of their singing voices at fre­quent intervals and shifted them after 12.5 years from soprano to alto, and gradually to tenor or baritone by 15 years as their voices dropped with growth of the larynx. He attributed failure to sing well to vocal abuse in early childhood and advocated early framing in breathing technique. Most experts stress the dangers for both boys and girls of singing in the mutation period and will not permit serious voice training to begin until 17 years with girls and 18-19 years with boys. Weiss pointed out that very few choirboys, possibly a mere 2%, ever turn into good adult singers and this he attributed to the irreparable damage that had occurred in adolescence. Some singing teachers and choirmasters have little knowledge of the anatomy and phys­iology of the vocal tract, do not instruct their pupils in the fundamentals of good voice production and fail to appreciate the dangers of the pubertal period.
The mutational period of the singing voice lasts much longer than that of the speaking voice and this also is not often understood and recognized. Growth in height may continue long after the voice has 'broken', and during this time the voice is vulnerable and cannot achieve its adult potential on account of physiological immaturity.






In a gist:

Structure
Infant
Adult
Position of larynx
Higher C2-C3(helps infant to suck and breath simultaneously)
Lower, C6-C7
Vocal tract shape
Short and funnel shaped
Long and tubular
Epiglottis
Omega
Broader
Thyroid angle
Boys:110
Girls:120
Males:90
Females:120
Vocal fold length
6-8mm
Males:17-23mm
Females:12-17mm
Vocal fold mucosal
Undifferentiated layers & immature vocal ligament
Complex layered

Laryngeal structure form

Soft and cartilaginous
Hyoid bone ossifies by 2 years, thyroid and cricoid ossify by 20-23 years, arytenoids ossify by late 30s
Thyroid cartilage
Broad & short
Becomes elongated and assumes proper shape
Mean fundamental frequency
440Hz
Males:120hz
Females:220Hz
Connection of thyroid cartilage to hyoid bone
Present
Absent
Length of membranous portion of the vocal folds
Same as the cartilaginous portion
Membranous portion is 2/3 of the cartilaginous portion


Summary
The cartilages of the infant larynx are much softer and more pliable than the adult. The thyroid laminae form of a semicircle, which during Growth becomes more and more angulated, until it approaches 90 degree in mature male and 120 degree in the female.
       Vocal fold: 3mm
       1st year : 5.5mm
       5th year:7.5mm
       6th year: 8mm
       15th year: 9.5 mm
Rate of growth Of vocal fold
       Growth rate of vocal fold/antero-posterior growth of the laryngeal cartilages
       At infancy=1 to 2.3
       At 1st year= 1 to 1.5
       Pubertal age???
       Male= 10mm
       Female=4mm

With increasing age ossification and calcification of the laryngeal cartilages begin to occur.


Comments

Popular posts from this blog

PERFORMANCE INTENSITY PHONETICALLY BALANCE (PIPB)

THE BUFFALO MODEL

SPECIAL TESTS OF DIAGNOSTIC AUDIOLOGY