BINAURAL FUSION TEST
Matzker (1959) developed
a binaural rest in which German two-syllable PB words were filtered through a
low-pass band (500—800 Hz) in one ear and a high-pass band (1815-2500 Hz) in
the other ear. The high-pass band was presented simultaneously with the
iow-pass band. Normal subjects are able to fuse the high-pass band and low-pass
band and recognize the word. That is, they integrate the information at the two
ears (summate binaurally). When only one of the bands (high- or low-pass) is
presented to an ear, normal subjects cannot recognize the word. This test is
considered a special case of dichotic speech tests since different but
complementary stimuli are presented simultaneously to the two ears. Matzker's
procedure involved first dichotic presentation, then diotic presentation (which
did not require binaural summation or fusion), followed again by dichotic
presentation. In normal-hearing subjects, performance in the dichotic mode is
essentially equivalent to performance in the diotic mode. Matzker hypothesized
that patients with brainstem pathology, particularly lower brainstem pathology,
would perform poorly on the dichotic mode of the binaural-fusion test; that
is, they would be unable to fuse the high-and low-pass bands of information.
Linden (1964) developed
a Swedish version of the Matzker binaural-fusion test based on Swedish
spondaic words. His low-pass band was from 560 to 715 Hz and his high-pass band
was from 1800 to 2200 Hz. Linden hypothesized that patients with various
central pathologies would obtain binaural-fusion test scores in the dichotic
mode lower than those when high-and low-pass bands were presented simultaneously
to one ear. The results did not bear out his hypothesis. Linden concluded
that his modification of the binaural-fusion test was not useful for the
detection of central auditory pathology.
In Ivey's (1969) version of
the binaural-fusion test, the low-pass band extended from 500 to 700 Hz and the
high-pass band extended from 1900 to 2100 Hz. The rejection rate of the filter
was 36 dB per octave. Two lists of 20 children's spondaic words were presented
with the low-pass band at 25 dB SL relative to the 500-Hz pure-tone threshold
and the high-pass band at 25 dB SL relative to the 2000-Hz threshold. The first
list was given with the low-pass band to one ear and the high-pass band to the
other ear. With the second list, the ears receiving the high- and low-pass
bands were reversed. Each item was assigned a value of 5%. Arbitrarily, the
binaural-fusion score is attributed to the ear in which the low-pass band is
presented. Ivey (1969) administered the binaural fusion test to a group of
normal-hearing listeners. The mean score for list 1 was higher than that for
list 2 (93.8% and 86.0%, respectively.
Willeford (1977) modified
the Ivey (1969) version of the binaural-fusion test, using the presentation of
30 rather than 25 dB SL (higher sensation levels if low scores are obtained at
30 dB SL). Willeford (1977), as noted by Lukas and Gendeur-Lukas (1985),
recommended that a 10% correction factor be added to score:; obtained for
list. 2. Monaural recognition is first obtained for each frequency band for
each ear. Then the binaural-fusion recognition scores are obtained with one
list presetted such that the right ear receives the low-frequency band and the
other list presented such that the left ear receives the low-frequency band.
Willeford (1977) reported that the mean binaural-fusion score for his group of
20 normal adults was 89% with a range of 75-100%. According
to Lynn and Gilroy (1975), normal binaural-fusion
scores usually surpass the highest monaural single-band score by at least 50%.
Lynn and Gilroy (1972) reported
that performance on the binaural-fusion test was abnormal in some of their
patients with temporai-lobe tumors. Nevertheless, in some cases there was
evidence of secondary brainstem compression. In a later study on 4 patients
with posterior temporal-lobe tumors, Lynn and Gilroy (1975)
reported that abnormal binaural-fusion test scores were obtained in 1 patient
and questionable bmaurai-fusion test scores were obtained in another patient.
Lynn and Gilroy (1975) also observed that the binaural-fusion test results were
abnormal in only 1 of their 6 patients with antero-inferior temporal-lobe
tumors, 0 of their 5 patients with superficial parietal-lobe tumors, and 1 of
their 8 patients with deep parietal-lobe tumors (with a questionable score in
another patient). These results suggest that the binaural-fusion test is not
sensitive to temporal-lobe or parietal-lobe tumors.
In the Smith and Resnick
(1972) version of the binaural-fusion test, monosyllables (CNC) were
employed. The low-frequency band was 360—890 Hz and the high-frequency band was
1750-2220 Hz. The test was presented under three conditions; (a) dichotic with
tbe low-pass band to one ear and the high-pass band to the opposite ear, (b)
diotic with high- and low-pass bands presented binaurally, and (c) the same
dichotic condition as in (a) except that the ears receiving the low- and
high-pass bands were reversed. Smith and Resnick (1972} found that performance
on the binaural-fusion test was similar across the three conditions in subjects
with normal hearing-thresh old levels, bilateral sensorineural hearing
impairment, and temporal-lobe pathology. In all 4 of their subjects with
brainstem pathology, performance in at least one of the two dichotic conditions
was reduced compared with that in the diotic condition. Smith and Resnick's
(1972) findings suggested that the binaural-fusion test could be used to detect
brainstem pathology. Palva and Jokinen (1975) reported that poor binaural-fusion
test scores (with good scores when the low-and high-pass bands were presented
monaurally) occurred frequently in patients with brainstem lesions of vascular
or traumatic etiology. On the other
hand, Lynn and Gilroy (1977) did not observe consistent
findings or patterns on the binaural-fusion test in patients with brainstem
lesions. Also, Musiek and Geurkink (1982), who used the Willeford (1977)
version of the binaural-fusion test, reported that only 30% of their 10
patients with brainstem lesions demonstrated abnormal binaural-fusion test
scores. These later reports suggest that the binaural-fusion test has a low
sensitivity to brainstem lesions, contrary to what was originally theorized.
Large-sample studies on the binaural-fusion test are required before this test
can be used clinically to detect brainstem dysfunction.
RAPIDLY ALTERNATING SPEECH PERCEPTION TEST
(RASP)
Bocca and Calearo (1963) developed
a swinging speech test following the method of Cherry
and Taylor (1954). In the swinging speech test, the message
alternates periodically between ears so that each ea.: receives half of the
message. Recognition of the entire message occurs only if the information to
the two ears is fused or resynthesized. Figure 13 illustrates the poor recognition
in each ear alone with complete recognition when the two ears receive the
complementary segments.
Bocca and Calearo (1963) found
that performance on the rapidly alternating speech perception (RASP) test was
unaffected by pathology of the auditory cortex. Since RASP performance was
affecred by some cases of diffuse cerebral pathology and many cases of
brainstem pathology, they suggested that the RASP test was dependent upon the
integrity of the centra] auditory nervous system at the level of the brainstem.
Lynn and Gilroy (1975) developed
their own English language version of the Bocca and Calearo swinging speech
test. The stimuli alternated every 300 ms. Normative data collected in their
laboratory revealed monaural scores ranging from 0-10% and binaural scores
from 95-100%. In a later study based on 18 normal
adults, Lynn and Gilroy (1977) obtained mean monaural
scores of 7.2 and 3.9% for the right and left ears, respectively, with standard
deviations of 9.0 and 5.1% for the right and left ears, respectively; in the
binaural condition, the mean score was 97.2% and the standard
deviation was 7.4%. The Willeford (1977) version of the RASP test differed from
the Lynn and Gilroy (1975) version essentially with respect
to the stimulus items. The presentation leve] is 30 dB SL relative to the
pure-tone average. Some investigators have recommended
BINAURAL RESYNTHESIS
RAPIDLY ALTERNATING SPEECH PERCEPTION
RE:
I' EE
YO GH
AF L
CH
LE:
LL
S
U Ri
T TER
UN
BIN:
I'LL SEE YOU RIGHT
AFTER LUNCH
figure 13 Binaural
resynthesis sample from the Rapidly Alternating Speech Perception Test.
the use of 40 dB SL
relative to the SRT (Tobin, 1985) or 50 dB SL relative to the SRT or PTA
(Musiek & Geurkink, 1982; Pinheiro, 1978). The procedure involves first presenting
the sentences monaurally to each ear (with a score for each ear) and then
presenting the sentences in the alternating mode with the two ears receiving
the complementary segments. Twenty sentences are presented, each sentence
having an assigned value of 5%. The sentences used in the RASP are shown in
Table VI. In the alternating mode, 10 sentences are presented so that the right
ear receives the first segment and 10 sentences are presented so that the left
ear receives the first segment. Willeford (1977) reported that the mean scores
for the RASP in a group of 20 normal adults was 99% with a range of 90-100%.
Lynn and Gilroy (1977) reported
that the RASP mean score was 100% in their group of 11 patients with right
temporal-lobe tumors, approximately 80% for their group of 11 patients with
left temporal-lobe tumors, approximately 92% for their group of 14 patients
with right parietal-lobe tumors, approximately 92% for their group of 10
patients with left parietal-lobe tumors, approximately 38% for their group of 6
patients with low pons cerebello-pontine-angle lesions, and approximately 84%
for their group of 9 patients with upper brainstem lesions. These results show that
the poorest RASP scores were obtained in patients with low brainstem lesions.
Musiek and Geurkink (1982) reported abnormal scores on the RASP test in
only 50% of their 10 patients with brainstem lesions.
Antonelli et al. (1987) obtained findings similar to those of
Musiek
Table VI Sentences
from the Rapidly Alternating Speech Perception Test"
SENTENCE
BEGINS______
TRACK
L
I. The children came home lace from school.
L
2. He likes to play and splash in the water.
R 3. She
is mad because the television was broken.
R
4. It rained very much for hours.
R
5. The wind almost blew the roof off.
L 6. What
time did you get in yesterday?
L
7. The school room was very large.
R
8. I took a picture of her with my camera.
L
9. What is your favorite television program?
R 10. Where have you
been all day?
L II. The
telephone rang for a real long time.
L 12. How did you put that
together again?
L 13. They went
to the park to play games.
L 14. Let
the dog in at the back door.
R 15. Answer the
telephone as soon as it rings.
R 16. Please
turn the radio up louder.
L 17. Are you
going to the office this morning?
L 18. That window is
very dirt}'.
R 19. His
father is on the police force.
R 20. I had bacon and
eggs for breakfast.
and Geurkink (1982).
Antonelli et al. (1987) reported that approximately 56% of
their 16 cases with intra-axial brainscem lesions had abnormal alternating
speech test scores whereas only approximately 6% of their 16
cases with temporal-lobe lesions had abnormal alternating speech scores.
SPEECH WITH ALTERNATE
MASKING INDEX (SWAMI)
Given
by Jerger
Check
for brainstem pathology
Present
speech signal binaurally and masking noise is alternated between 2 ears.
Here
also binaural SIS is founding presence of noise.
There
is a noise burst in both ears alternatively so our ear will be receiving masked
signal and other receiving good signal.
Normally
normal subjects will get 100% score because of binaural integration.
In
subjects with brain stem lesion results will be poorer.
Signalis
presented at 40-50dBSL
0dB
SNR or less is used.
Prefebarly
PB list or monosyllable are used.
Jerger
originally used PB words for this test.
MASKING LEVEL DIFFERENCE
TEST
Binaural advantages in
detection of signals in masking noise have been investigated in various
clinical populations. These binaural advantages are also known as binaural
release from masking, masking level differences (MLDs), or binaural unmasking.
The mechanism for extraction of the signal from a background of noise is
believed to be analysis of interaural phase (time) differences (Durlach &c Colburn,
3978).
Consider the case of a
person who has a noise (N) and a signal (S) such as speech presented to one
ear. This condition, which reflects monotic presentation of the signal and
noise, has been represented in the literature as SmNm. The condition in which
the signal is presented to one ear but the noise is presented to both ears with
a 180° interaural phase difference is represented as SmNπ. The condition in
which the signal is presented to one ear but the noise is presented to both
ears with a 0° interaural phase difference is represented as SmNo. The
condition in which the signal is presented to both ears with a 0° interaural
phase difference and the noise is presented to both ears with a 0○ interaural
phase difference is represented as SoKo. The condition in which the signal is
presented to both ears with a 180° interaural phase difference and the noise is
presented to both ears with a 0° interaural phase difference is represented
as SהּNo. The
condition in which the signal is presented to both ears with a 0° interaural
phase difference and the noise is presented to both ears with a 180° interaural
phase difference is represented as S0Nהּ. The condition in which the signal is presented
to both ears with a 180° interaura] phase difference and the noise is presented
to both ears with a 180° phase difference is represented as SהּNהּ.
Figure 14 illustrates some
of these experimental conditions. The S0N0 condition is hcmophasic since the
noise and signals are in phase. The SoNהּ, SהּNo, and SהּNהּ are antiphasic conditions since the noises
and/or the signals are out of phase.
According to Diercks and
Jeffress (1962), the SmNm, S0N0, and SהּNהּ produce
the poorest thresholds. The
SoNהּ and SהּNo produce
the best thresholds. This situation is illustrated by the magnitude of the
smile or frown in Figure 14. In normal-hearing persons, the threshold for a
signal in the SmNo or SmNהּ condition
is better than that in the SmNm condition. Also, the threshold for a signal in
the SoNהּ or SהּNo condition is better than that in the SoNo
condition. These improvements reflect release from masking. The MLD can be
obtained by subtracting the threshold level for the signal in the SmNo or SmNהּ condition
from the threshold level for the signal in the SmNm condition. The MLD can also
be obtained by subtracting the threshold level for the signal in the SoNהּ or SהּNo condition
from the threshold level for the signal in the SoNo condition.
The MLD for speech is most
commonly measured by obtaining the spondee recognition threshold in the SoNo
condition (reference condition) and SoNהּ or SהּNo condition.
The size of the MLD is defined as the difference between the SRT in the
bomophasic condition and that in the antiphasic (SהּNo or
SoNהּ) condition. The intensity of the broad-band noise is usually
presented at 80 dB SPL (Lynn, Gilroy, Taylor, & Leiser, 1981; Olsen &
Noffsinger, 1976; Olsen, Noffsinger, & Carhart, 1976). Some two-channel
audiometers (e.g., GSI-10) have a network that allows phase reversal of either the
noise or test signal. Adaptors with the mixer and phase shift capability are
commercially available for use with two-channel audiometer and can be purchased
from some companies.
Olsen and Noffsinger (1976) obtained
mean spondee MLDs of 7.3 dB for the SהּNo antiphasic
condition and 6.9 dB for the SoNהּ antiphasic
condition when the reference condition was SoNo in their group of 12
normal-hearing subjects. Olsen et al. (1976) obtained a mean
spondee MLD of 8.3 and 6.9 dB for the SהּNo and SoNהּ conditions,
respectively, in their group of 50 normal-hearing subjects. Since only 6%
obtained spondee MLDs less than or equal to 5 dB for the SהּNo condition
and less than or equal to 3 dB for the SoNהּ condition,
the cutoff MLD for normalcy below which the MLD was abnormally reduced was 5 dB
for the former condition and 3 dB for the latter condition. Lynn et
al. (1981) reported that the mean MLD in their group of normal-hearing
subjects was 12.2 dB for the SהּNo antiphasic
condition and 10.2 dB for the SoNהּ antiphasic condition; the standard
deviation was 1.1 dB for both antiphasic conditions and the range was 10-14 dB
for the SהּNo antiphasic
condition and 8-12 dB for the SoNהּ antiphasic
condition.
Cullen and Thompson (1974) reported
that the MLD score (% Michigan CNC words correct in SoNהּ or SהּNo condition
minus % Michigan CNC words correct in the SoNo condition) in 4 patients with
anterior temporal-lobe lesions involving the superior gyrus fell in the range
defining one standard deviation above and below the mean in normal-hearing
listeners. Cullen and Thompson (1974) concluded that an intact auditory
association was not essential for release of masking to occur. They also
concluded that the release from masking phenomenon was probably mediated at
subthalamic levels. Lynn et al. (1981) reported that the mean
MLD detection threshold in their group of 12 patients with cerebral lesions did
not differ significantly from that of the normal subjects for both SהּNo and SoNהּ antiphasic
conditions. Olsen et al. (1976) reported that abnormally small
spondee MI.Ds were obtained in only 5% of their 20 patients with cortical
lesions for the SהּNo anti-phasic
condition and 0% of these patients for the SoNהּ antiphasic
condition. Olsen et al. (1976) concluded that the MLD was
mediated at subcorcical levels. Noffsinger (1982) reported that the spondee MLD
(SoNo - SהּNo)
was abnormally small (less than 6 dB) in only 9% of their 67 cases with
temporal-lobe lesions. The results of these investigations on the MLD in
temporal-lobe lesions show that the MLD is generally unaffected by cortical
lesions.
Olsen and Noffsinger (1976) reported
that 58% of their 12 patients with multiple sclerosis and other pathologies of
the midbrain and/or brainstern region had abnormlly small spondee MLDs of less
than 6 dB for the SהּNo antiphasic condition and/or less
than 4 dB in the SoNהּ antiphasic condition. They concluded
that the MLD was often impaired by central-nervous system pathologies
associated with multiple sclerosis or other midbrain and/or brainstem lesions.
Noffsinger (1982) reported that 52% of their 114 patients with brainstem
lesions and normal-hearing sensitivity had spondee MLDs (SoNo minus SהּNo) that
were abnormally reduced (<6 dB). Noffsinger (1982) concluded that abnormally
reduced MLDs in persons with normal hearing-thresholds signaled the presence of
brainstem pathology. Lynn et al. (1981) reported that the
mean MLDs for the speech-detection threshold did not differ significantly
among the normal, cerebral,
and upper pons/ midbrain/thalamic groups. The mean MLD
was significantly reduced in the pontomedullary group compared with the
normal, cerebral, and upper pons/midbrain/thaiamic groups. Lynn et
al. (1981) concluded that the pontomedullary region of the brainstem
(lower brainstem) mediates that MLD phenomenon and suggested that the region of
the superior olivary nuclei was a likely site for mediation of the MLD since it
was the most caudal anatomical site in the brainstem where there was integration
of information from the two ears. The hypothesis of Lynn et al. (1981)
was substantiated by the findings of Noffsinger, Martinez, and Schaefer
(1982). Noffsinger et al. (1982) found that abnormally small
MLDs and abnormally elevated or absent acoustic-reflex
thresholds occurred in
patients with brainstem lesions when abnormalities in the
brainstem auditory-evoked potentials began with wave I, II, or III but normal
MLDs and acoustic-reflex thresholds occurred in patients with brainstem lesions
when abnormalities in the brainstem auditory-evoked potentials began with wave
IV or V. Since waves I and II probably arise from the eighth cranial nerve, and
the primary contribution to wave III is the superior olivary complex according
to Britt and Rossi (1980) and Rossi and Britt (1980) or the cochlear nuclei
according to Moller, Jannetta, and Moller (1981), Noffsinger et
al. (1982) proposed that cochlear nuclei and/or the superior olivary
complex may be responsible for the binaural interaction processes involved in
the MLD phenomenon. Since abnormally reduced spondee MLDs occur in
patient with eighth-nerve
tumors even when there is
normal-hearing sensitivity and symmetrical audiometric configurations
(Noffsinger, 1982; Olsen et al., 1976), the presence of
a reduced MLD in persons with bilateral normal-hearing thresholds
and symmetrical audiometric configurations does not differentiate between
eighth nerve and brainstem sites of lesion. The spondee MLD is associated with
a higher sensitivity than the 500-Hz MLD to brainstem pathology (Olsen &
Noffsinger, 1976; Noffsinger, 1982).
Nevertheless, since the hit
rate for the spondee MLD appears not to be substantially higher than
chance, the spondee MLD test does not appear to hold great promise as a
clinical tool for the detection of brainstem dysfunction in normal-hearing
persons.
Several
studies have shown reduced speech MLDs in patients with multiple sclerosis
(Hannley, Jerger, & Rivera, 1983; Olsen & Noffsinger, 1976; Olsen et
al, 1976).
Comments
Post a Comment